Negative observational learning might play a limited role in the cultural evolution of technology | Scientific Reports – Nature.com

Lewis, H. M. & Laland, K. N. Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 21712180 (2012).

Google Scholar

Tennie, C., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 364, 24052415 (2009).

PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Burdett, E. R. R., Dean, L. G. & Ronfard, S. A diverse and flexible teaching toolkit facilitates the human capacity for cumulative culture. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 9, 807818 (2018).

PubMed Google Scholar

Reindl, E., Gwilliams, A. L., Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L. & Tennie, C. Skills and motivations underlying childrens cumulative cultural learning: Case not closed. Palgrave Commun. 6, 19 (2020).

Google Scholar

Legare, C. H. & Nielsen, M. Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 688699 (2015).

PubMed Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition (Harvard University Press, 1999).

Google Scholar

Horner, V. & Whiten, A. Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim. Cogn. 8, 164181 (2005).

PubMed Google Scholar

Kenward, B., Karlsson, M. & Persson, J. Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 12391246 (2011).

PubMed Google Scholar

Hoehl, S. et al. Over-imitation: A review and appraisal of a decade of research. Dev. Rev. 51, 90108 (2019).

Google Scholar

Heyes, C. M. & Frith, C. D. The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science 344, 1243091 (2014).

PubMed Google Scholar

Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 148153 (2009).

PubMed Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. & Carpenter, M. Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 10, 121125 (2007).

PubMed Google Scholar

Moll, H. & Tomasello, M. Cooperation and human cognition: The Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 362, 639648 (2007).

PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Muthukrishna, M. et al. Innovation in the collective brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 137148 (2016).

Google Scholar

Heyes, C. Blackboxing: Social learning strategies and cultural evolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371, 1693 (2016).

Google Scholar

Morgan, T. J. H., Rendell, L. E., Ehn, M., Hoppitt, W. & Laland, K. N. The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 653662 (2012).

CAS Google Scholar

Mesoudi, A. An experimental simulation of the copy-successful-individuals cultural learning strategy: adaptive landscapes, producerscrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 350363 (2008).

Google Scholar

Laland, K. N. Social learning strategies. Anim. Learn. Behav. 32, 414 (2004).

Google Scholar

Schlag, K. H. Which one should I imitate? J. Math. Econ. 31, 493522 (1999).

MathSciNet MATH Google Scholar

Smith, K., Kirby, S. & Brighton, H. Iterated learning: A framework for the emergence of language. Artif. Life 9, 371386 (2003).

PubMed Google Scholar

Zwirner, E. & Thornton, A. Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: Teaching is useful but not essential. Sci. Rep. 5, 18 (2015).

Google Scholar

Haidle, M. N. & Schlaudt, O. Where does cumulative culture begin? A plea for a sociologically informed perspective. Biol. Theory 15, 161174 (2020).

Google Scholar

Saldana, C., Fagot, J., Kirby, S., Smith, K. & Claidire, N. High-fidelity copying is not necessarily the key to cumulative cultural evolution: A study in monkeys and children. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 286, 20190729 (2019).

Google Scholar

King, A. J., Cheng, L., Starke, S. D. & Myatt, J. P. Is the true wisdom of the crowd to copy successful individuals? Biol. Lett. 8, 197200 (2012).

PubMed Google Scholar

Hewlett, B. S. & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. Cultural transmission among aka pygmies. Am. Anthropol. 88, 922934 (1986).

Google Scholar

Jimnez, . V. & Mesoudi, A. Prestige-biased social learning: Current evidence and outstanding questions. Palgrave Commun. 5, 112 (2019).

Google Scholar

Henrich, J. & Gil-White, F. J. The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 165196 (2001).

CAS PubMed Google Scholar

McElreath, R. et al. Beyond existence and aiming outside the laboratory: Estimating frequency-dependent and pay-off-biased social learning strategies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 35153528 (2008).

PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Nakahashi, W. A mathematical model of cultural interactions between modern and archaic humans. In Dynamics of Learning in Neanderthals and Modern Humans Vol. 1 (eds Akazawa, T. et al.) 255263 (Springer, 2013).

Olsson, A. & Phelps, E. A. Social learning of fear. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 10951102 (2007).

CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Galef, B. G. Social identification of toxic diets by Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). J. Comp. Psychol. 100, 331334 (1986).

PubMed Google Scholar

Ferrari, M. C. O. & Chivers, D. P. First documentation of cultural transmission of predator recognition by Larval Amphibians. Ethology 113, 621627. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01362.x (2007).

Article Google Scholar

Scalise Sugiyama, M. Lions and tigers and bears: Predators as a folklore universal. In Anthropology and Social History: Heuristics in the Study of Literature (eds Friedrich, H. Jannidis, F. Klein, U. Mellmann, K. Metzger, S. & Willem, M.) 319331 (Mentis, 1996).

Nakawake, Y. & Sato, K. Systematic quantitative analyses reveal the folk-zoological knowledge embedded in folktales. Palgrave Commun. 5, 161 (2019).

Google Scholar

Wilks, C. E. H., Rafetseder, E., Renner, E., Atkinson, M. & Caldwell, C. A. Cognitive prerequisites for cumulative culture are context-dependent: Childrens potential for ratcheting depends on cue longevity. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 204, 105031 (2021).

PubMed Google Scholar

Want, S. C. & Harris, P. L. Learning from other peoples mistakes: Causal understanding in learning to use a tool. Child Dev. 72, 431443 (2001).

CAS Article PubMed Google Scholar

Henrich, J. Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive lossesThe Tasmanian case. Am. Antiq. 69, 197214 (2004).

Google Scholar

Kobayashi, Y. & Aoki, K. Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. Theor. Popul. Biol. 82, 3847 (2012).

PubMed MATH Google Scholar

Mesoudi, A. & OBrien, M. J. The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. Am. Antiq. 73, 627644 (2008).

Google Scholar

Mesoudi, A. & OBrien, M. The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology II: An agent-based computer simulation. Am. Antiq. 73, 627644 (2008).

Google Scholar

Mesoudi, A. An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: Payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but underused. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 334342 (2011).

Google Scholar

Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Murray, K. & Lu, H. J. Higher frequency of social learning in China than in the West shows cultural variation in the dynamics of cultural evolution. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 282, 20142209 (2014).

Google Scholar

Thompson, B. & Griffiths, T. L. Human biases limit cumulative innovation. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 288, 20202752 (2021).

Google Scholar

Garcia-Retamero, R., Takezawa, M. & Gigerenzer, G. Does imitation benefit cue order learning? Exp. Psychol. 56, 307320 (2009).

PubMed Google Scholar

Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., ABC Research Group. Simple Heuristics that Make Us Smart (Oxford University Press, 1999).

Google Scholar

Hastie, R. & Dawes, R. M. Rational Choice in an Uncertain World: The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Making (Sage, 2001).

Google Scholar

Acerbi, A., Tennie, C. & Mesoudi, A. Social learning solves the problem of narrow-peaked search landscapes: Experimental evidence in humans. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3, 160215 (2016).

ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Higashi, M., Suzuki, R. & Arita, T. The role of social learning in the evolution on a rugged fitness landscape. Front. Phys. 6, 19 (2018).

Google Scholar

Derex, M. & Boyd, R. Social information can potentiate understanding despite inhibiting cognitive effort. Sci. Rep. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-28306-z (2018).

Lyons, D. E., Damrosch, D. H., Lin, J. K., Macris, D. M. & Keil, F. C. The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 11581167 (2011).

PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S. & Hopper, L. M. Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 24172428 (2009).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Derex, M., Bonnefon, J. F., Boyd, R. & Mesoudi, A. Causal understanding is not necessary for the improvement of culturally evolving technology. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 446452 (2019).

Read the original post:

Negative observational learning might play a limited role in the cultural evolution of technology | Scientific Reports - Nature.com

Related Posts

Comments are closed.